The happiness paradox: your friends are happier than you

EPJ Data Science, May 2017

Most individuals in social networks experience a so-called Friendship Paradox: they are less popular than their friends on average. This effect may explain recent findings that widespread social network media use leads to reduced happiness. However the relation between popularity and happiness is poorly understood. A Friendship paradox does not necessarily imply a Happiness paradox where most individuals are less happy than their friends. Here we report the first direct observation of a significant Happiness Paradox in a large-scale online social network of 39,110 Twitter users. Our results reveal that popular individuals are indeed happier and that a majority of individuals experience a significant Happiness paradox. The magnitude of the latter effect is shaped by complex interactions between individual popularity, happiness, and the fact that users strongly cluster by similar level of happiness. Our results indicate that the topology of online social networks, combined with how happiness is distributed in some populations, may be associated with significant psycho-social effects.

A PDF file should load here. If you do not see its contents the file may be temporarily unavailable at the journal website or you do not have a PDF plug-in installed and enabled in your browser.

Alternatively, you can download the file locally and open with any standalone PDF reader:

https://link.springer.com/content/pdf/10.1140%2Fepjds%2Fs13688-017-0100-1.pdf

The happiness paradox: your friends are happier than you

Bollen et al. EPJ Data Science The happiness paradox: your friends are happier than you Johan Bollen 0 Bruno Gonçalves 2 Ingrid van de Leemput 1 Guangchen Ruan 0 0 School of Informatics and Computing, Indiana University , Bloomington, IN , USA 1 Environmental Sciences, Wageningen University , Wageningen , The Netherlands 2 Center for Data Science, New York University , New York , USA Most individuals in social networks experience a so-called Friendship Paradox: they are less popular than their friends on average. This effect may explain recent findings that widespread social network media use leads to reduced happiness. However the relation between popularity and happiness is poorly understood. A Friendship paradox does not necessarily imply a Happiness paradox where most individuals are less happy than their friends. Here we report the first direct observation of a significant Happiness Paradox in a large-scale online social network of 39,110 Twitter users. Our results reveal that popular individuals are indeed happier and that a majority of individuals experience a significant Happiness paradox. The magnitude of the latter effect is shaped by complex interactions between individual popularity, happiness, and the fact that users strongly cluster by similar level of happiness. Our results indicate that the topology of online social networks, combined with how happiness is distributed in some populations, may be associated with significant psycho-social effects. subjective well-being; social media; social network; friendship paradox; sentiment analysis; natural language processing; data science 1 Introduction We are a profoundly social species []. The ability to establish face-to-face, physical, relationships in a rich social environment is paramount to our happiness and individual well-being [–]. However, technology is now playing an increasing role in forming our networks of social relationships. Nearly /th of the world’s population and over /rd of the US population [] use some form of social media which enables individuals to maintain virtual social networks that extend beyond geographical, economic, cultural, and linguis Evidence has been accumulating that online social networking is associated with elevated levels of loneliness, anxiety, displeasure, and dissatisfaction [–]. The reason for this apparent contradiction is unknown, but it may be found in universal social network connectivity patterns. Surprisingly, measured in number of connections, most people will have fewer friends than their own friends do on average. This phenomenon, commonly referred to as the Friendship Paradox [, ], has been attributed to an inherent structural bias in social network that favors popular individuals: they are by definition more likely to belong to someones social circle, thereby elevating local levels of popularity. One may © The Author(s) 2017. This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. Figure 1 A Happiness paradox, like a Friendship paradox, could result from skewed degree distributions. (A1) Like the examples above, most social networks are characterized by very skewed degree distributions: a few individuals have very many connections, while most individuals have only few connections. The number of connections are marked within each node. Those with many connections are by definition more likely to be someone’s friend. As a result their higher number of connections can increase the average degree of given friendship neighborhoods throughout the network (marked above each node) leading to a Friendship paradox (red nodes) in which most individuals nodes are less popular than the average of their friends. (A2) When popular individuals are also more likely to be happy, their Happiness becomes more prevalent, raising average happiness levels throughout the friendship circles in the network. A Happiness paradox may result in which most individuals are less happy than their own friends on average. Individuals may cluster based on their Happiness or even the degree to which they experience a Happiness paradox. speculate that if an individual compares their own popularity to that of their friends, this effect may, in some cases, lead to increased levels of dissatisfaction (see Figure ). The effects of this Friendship Paradox may extend beyond popularity. If popular individuals tend to be happier, their elevated happiness will become more prevalent as well. This may in turn lead to a Happiness Paradox, where most individuals are less happy than their friends on average (see Figure ). In fact, the latter may contribute more directly to the negative psycho-social effects of social networking since it affects how individuals assess their own Subjective Well-being, i.e. general happiness or life satisfaction [, ], relative to that of others [, ]. At this point, however, it has not been established whether () popular individuals are indeed happier and () a Happiness Paradox does in fact occur in social networks. Given the magnitude of social media adoption, these are questions of global importance that may affect the well-being of billions of individuals. 2 Methods Here we present the first large-scale longitudinal study of happiness and popularity levels for a network of , Twitter users that are connected by ‘friendship’ relations. To generate a friendship network among Twitter users we start with an initial set of ,, randomly chosen users (years -), for which we downloaded the full list of users that they ‘follow’ or that they are ‘followed’ by []. Reciprocal ‘Follow’ and ‘Following’ ties are taken as an indication of a friendship and mutual interaction relation between the two individuals []. We selected the Largest Connected Component of the resulting network. The resulting network consists of , individuals that share ,, edges. We automatically assess each individual’s Subjective Well-Being (SWB), on a scale of [–, +] according to a procedure outlined in []. For each user we collected the , most recently submitted tweets, serving as a comprehensive longitudinal record pertaining to the individual user. We subject this entire timeline, not its individual tweets, to a sentiment analysis [] based on the OpinionFinder (OF) subjectivity lexicon []. The procedure consists of counting the number of words that are members of either the set of highly positive or highly negative words in the OF subjectivity lexicon, and calculating their fractional difference (number of positive words-negative words over all OF words in the timeline). Aggregating this information for all Tweets in an individual Twitter record, we determine the individual’s overall Subjective Well-Being. The OF toolkit, although different from the above described application of its subjectivity lexicon, was ranked th out of  tools in a large-scale survey of - and -way sentiment classification tasks against a variety of data sets []. Its accuracy in scoring individual tweets ranged from . to ., mostly approximating, and sometimes exceeding, that of top rated tools such as VADER or AFINN, with coverage levels from . to ., depending on the specific data set against which OF was tested. We stress that our procedure uses the OF Subjectivity Lexicon against an entire  month timeline of up to , tweets by the same individual Twitter user, not individual tweets. Hence the reported coverage values are expected to be a significant underestimation relative to our application. In addition, we exclude users with SWB values of exactly zero. Finally, we restrict our analysis to individuals with more than  friends in order to exclude individuals with excessively low social activity and those that have non-zero SWB, i.e. that have shared some subjective information. This reduces our final cohort to , subjects that are connected by reciprocal friendship relations, have at least  friends, and have non-zero SWB values. We quantify an individuals ‘Happiness’ as their SWB value and quantify their ‘Popularity’ as their number of network friends counted in our bidirectional social network of reciprocal connections. Simply put, we deem an individual ‘Happy’ when they have high SWB values and ‘Popular’ when they have many friends. The Happiness and Popularity values of all subjects are then used to determine: . the fraction of individuals that has lower popularity than their friends on average (P); . the fraction of individuals that has lower happiness than their friends on average (H); . the correlation between individual happiness and popularity, R (Happiness, Popularity). We assess the magnitude of the Friendship Paradox in our network by calculating the fraction of the number of users ui ∈ U whose Popularity, denoted D(ui) (degree of ui) is lower than the average Popularity D¯ of their nearest neighbors (or ‘friends’) Nui ⊂ U vs. the total number of individuals in the network U . This yields the magnitude of the Friendship Paradox as: P = H = The magnitude of the Happiness Paradox can be obtained in a similar way to how we measure the Friendship Paradox. We simply calculate the fraction of users ui ∈ U whose Happiness, denoted H(ui) (SWB value of ui), is lower than the average Happiness H¯ of their nearest neighbors, Nui , vs. the total number of individuals in the network U : A Friendship or Happiness Paradox for our sample is indicated by P and H values larger than %, i.e. a majority of individuals have lower Popularity or Happiness than their friends, on average. To assess the correlation between Happiness and Popularity we calculate Pearson’s R between the SWB values and log(degree) of all subjects in our cohort. The use of log(degree) is meant to compensate for the very skewed distribution of degree values in our network. We assess the robustness of our results by performing a bootstrapping procedure in which we randomly sample % of subjects and their network connections with replacement , times to assess the distribution of our paradox indicators for different samples of our network. Furthermore, we validate the statistical significance of our results by comparing them to a null-model where we reshuffle the SWB values across all individuals in our network. In this way, we are able to maintain the same identical distribution of SWB values and network structure, while completely eliminating any possible correlation that might be present. The null-model was bootstrapped , times and, as expected, it eliminated the Happiness paradox. Finally, we determine the sensitivity of our results to reductions of the sample due to the ‘minimum friends threshold’ by recalculating all Happiness Paradox magnitudes for values of the threshold ranging from  to . 3 Results As shown in Table , we find that P = ., % CI [., .], indicating a very significant Friendship Paradox across all subjects, meaning that the great majority of users are less popular than their friends are on average. We also find a modest but robust value of H = .%, % CI [., .], indicating the presence of a Happiness Paradox. Hence a majority of subjects is indeed less happy than their friends on average. Our null-model indicates the absence of a Happiness paradox when the effects of network structure on Happiness levels are removed by random re-assignment. The lower magnitude of the Happiness Paradox could result from the rather low, yet robust, correlation between Happiness and Popularity (Pearson’s R = ., % CI [., .]). As shown in Figure  the joint distribution of individual Happiness levels and mean neighbor happiness in our sample is distinctly bi-modal. There exists some evidence that Subjective Well-Being itself can be distributed in a bimodal fashion across several cultures and nations [] matching previous observations by []. In this case, bi-modality also occurs at the level of our friendship network which separates subjects into  distinct groups: Happy subjects with Happy friends (the ‘Happy’ group) and Unhappy subjects with Unhappy friends (the ‘Unhappy’ group). This result follows earlier reports of happiness being homophilic or assortative in social networks [, , ]. Note that this phenomenon Table 1 Magnitude of Friendship Paradox, Happiness Paradox (compared to null-model produced by randomly re-assigning SBW values across all subjects), and Happiness-Popularity correlation coefficient (Pearson’s R) for all subjects (N = 39,110) Figure 2 Overview of the magnitude of the Happiness and Friendship paradox for the sample of Twitter subjects; individuals positioned above the diagonal experience a Happiness or Friendship paradox. (B1) Happiness Paradox: Distribution of individual Happiness (x-axis) vs. average Happiness of one’s friend’s average (y-axis). Happiness is measured in terms of longitudinal Subjective Well-Being (SWB) scores. Subjects above the red paradox line experience lower happiness (SWB) than their friends’ average. The distribution of SWB scores places a majority of subjects well above the diagonal Paradox line. Ellipses indicate the boundaries of 2 Gaussian Mixture Model components used to demarcate a Happy (red) and Unhappy (blue) groups of subjects. Paradox magnitudes are expressed in terms of the percentage of users who experience lower happiness than their friends. The 95% confidence intervals are calculated by a 5,000-fold bootstrapping of a 10% sample to determine the sensitivity of our results to random network sampling variations. (B2) and (B3) Friendship Paradox: Distribution of individual Popularity (x-axis) vs. average Popularity of one’s Friends (y-axis). Popularity is measured in terms of log(degree) in the Friendship network. Subjects above the red paradox line experience lower popularity than their friends on average. As shown, we find significant Happiness and Friendship Paradoxes for all users, but Happy users experience a stronger Friendship Paradox whereas Unhappy users experience a stronger Happiness Paradox. is not the result of the distribution of our SWB values, but is determined by network connections; i.e. unhappy people tend to have unhappy friends and happy people tend to have Since a Happiness Paradox specifically compares individual happiness to the average happiness of one’s friends, this homophilic bi-modality must be factored into our analysis. By performing a separate analysis for Happy and Unhappy groups of users, we attempt to equalize the effects of neighbor happiness across the two groups. As shown in Figure  we use a Gaussian Mixture Model (GMM) to demarcate our Happy and Unhappy groups. We determine the location and distribution of two separate Gaussian components in the distribution of individual happiness vs. mean friend happiness and demarcate both groups by simply determining whether the SWB value of a subject and the mean SWB values of their neighbors fall within  standard deviations from the center of either one of the components (illustrated by the  ellipses in Figure ). We thereby split our sample in  groups of subjects: a group of Happy individuals with Happy friends Figure 3 Bootstrapped estimates of the correlation between Happiness and Popularity, and the magnitude of the Friendship and Happiness Paradox for Happy and Unhappy subjects. Top: Estimated Pearson’s R correlation coefficients (95% Confidence Intervals in brackets) between individual Happiness (Subjective Well-Being) vs. individual Popularity (log degree) for All subjects: 0.109 [0.077, 0.140], Happy group: 0.126 [0.081, 0.171], and unhappy group: -0.047 [–0.08, –0.013]. Middle: Distribution of Friendship Paradox values for all subjects 0.943 [0.937, 0.949], happy group: 0.958 [0.951, 0.964], and unhappy group 0.888 [0.869, 0.906]. Bottom: Distribution of Happiness Paradox values for all subjects: 0.585 [0.581, 0.589], happy group: 0.578 [0.573, 0.582], and unhappy group 0.666 [0.657, 0.674]. cedure assumes a Gaussian density distribution which roughly matches the quantiles of the empirical density as shown by the contour lines of Figure , so that both Gaussian components capture % of our sample. These groups are well-separated in the underlying social network. Of all edges adjacent to individuals in the Happy group only .% connect to individuals in the Unhappy group. Vice versa, of all edges adjacent to individuals in the Unhappy group only .% connect to individuals in the Happy group. We re-run our analysis for the Happy and Unhappy groups separately. The results are summarized in Figures  and . These results reveal that the Happy group experiences a strong Friendship Paradox but a weak, yet very robust Happiness Paradox. The Unhappy group experiences a weaker Friendship Paradox, but a significantly stronger Happiness Paradox than the Happy group, in spite of subjects being surrounded by less Happy friends. To determine whether the strong Happiness Paradox for the unhappy group, in spite of its lower correlation between Popularity and Happiness, may be related to interpersonal effects, we examine the relation between individual happiness and the average happiness of ones neighbors. As visually indicated by the distribution of individuals in Figure , the strength of the relationship between a subjects’ Happiness and the average Happiness of their friends may differ between the Happy and Unhappy group. The results of a linear regression predicting mean neighbor SWB from a user’s own SWB values match the visual tilt of the separate GMM components. For the Happy group we find F(, ,) = ,, p < . with an adjusted R = .. The resulting regression equation is Mean Neighbor SWB = . + . (own SWB). For the Unhappy Group we find F(, ,) = ,, p < . with an adjusted R = .. Here, the resulting regression equation is Mean Neighbor SWB = . + . (own SWB). An ANCOVA analysis to predict Average Neighbor SWB from the interaction between a user’s own SWB and their membership of the Happy or Unhappy group results in F(, ,) = ,, p < . with an adjusted R = .. On the basis of this last result we reject the null-hypothesis that the regression slopes between own SWB and mean neighbor SWB are equal between the Happy and Unhappy group. This outcome suggests that unhappy users may be more strongly affected by the lower happiness of their friends, possibly explaining why this group exhibits a stronger Happiness Paradox in the absence of a strong correlation between Popularity and Happiness. However, we caution that further investigation is warranted on this matter. Finally, to determine the sensitivity of our analysis to the choice of our minimum neighbors threshold (the minimum number of neighbors a user must have to be included in our sample), we recalculate Happiness paradox values for all threshold values between  and . We use the same GMM component locations for every calculation to provide an equal basis for comparison. As shown in Table  and Figure , we find that requiring a minimum threshold of  neighbors for each individual in our data constitutes a signif≥Neighbors 1 5 10 15 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 90 100 125 150 175 200 Figure 4 Minimum neighbor threshold vs. percentage of sample retained. In our calculation of the Happiness Paradox we apply a minimum neighbor threshold for each user, i.e. a minimum number of neighbors the individual must have to be included in our sample, to (1) reduce the chances of including defunct, automated, or particularly socially inactive users and (2) ensure that a mean neighbor degree or SWB is calculated on the basis of no less than 15 data points. This threshold leads to a reduction of our sample. This graph visualizes the magnitude of the reduction at chosen threshold value of 15 and all other values between 1 and 200. Values provided in Table 2. Figure 5 Happiness paradox values for Happy and Unhappy group at different minimum neighbor threshold values, with 95% confidence intervals. Overlayed are the remaining sample size for the same threshold values. The gray area indicates paradox value below 50%, i.e. no happiness paradox. We find a significant happiness paradox, under all but the most extreme values of the minimum neighbor threshold. icant reduction of our sample, but we still retain more than % of the entire sample. In exchange for this reduction, we attempt to increase the chances of removing noise caused by socially inactive, and possibly automated or defunct accounts. The value of this threshold is notably much lower than recent estimations of the online Dunbar number []. The results of our sensitivity analysis, shown in Figure , indicate that the magnitude of the happiness paradox is largely independent from our choice of threshold, with the exception of extremely large values, i.e. where less than % of the original sample remains and only individuals with more than  friends are included, thus approaching the Dunbar limit []. We find a significant happiness paradox for the Unhappy group for all values of the minimum neighbor threshold indicating that this result is largely independent from 4 Conclusion We find that a majority of both Happy and Unhappy social media users will experience a significant Popularity and Happiness Paradox, i.e. they will be less popular and less happy than their friends on average. One may speculate that previous observations reporting decreased happiness among social media users may be associated with a widespread inflated perception of the happiness of one’s friends. Although happy and unhappy groups of subjects are both affected by a significant happiness paradox, unhappy subjects seem most strongly affected. This is counter-intuitive for two reasons. First, the correlation between happiness and popularity is lowest for individuals in the unhappy group. A happiness paradox can result from a friendship paradox when popularity and happiness are correlated, since more popular and thus more prevalent individuals will increase the average happiness of ones circle of friends. As a result, the unhappy group, with the lowest correlation between popularity and happiness, should experience the lowest happiness paradox. Second, the strong assortativity of happiness in our social network reduces the prevalence of happy subjects in the social network circle of unhappy subjects. Therefore, it should be easier for individuals in this group to surpass the average happiness of their friends. Our results show that neither is the case. We speculate that a possible explanation may lie in the stronger relation between the happiness of individuals in this group and the overall happiness of their friends. This effect may point to an alternate origin for the occurrence of a Happiness paradox; instead of resulting from the greater prevalence of popular and happy individuals, in some cases, a happiness paradox may result from the complex social interactions between individuals and their friends, e.g. through mood contagion [–] and potentially verbal commiseration and mirroring. Our results suggest that social media use may be associated with increased levels of social dissatisfaction and unhappiness if individuals are subject to unfavorable comparison between their own happiness and popularity to those of their friends. Happy social media users may think their friends are much more popular and slightly happier than they are while unhappy social media users will likely have unhappy friends that will still seem much happier and more popular than they are on average. However, our study has limitations. First, the assessment of Subjective Well-Being from social media using text analysis algorithms may not be perfectly reliable. However, given the large number of individuals in our dataset, the absence of consistent directional bias, and the magnitudes of the observed effects, we expect this will not affect the validity of our observations. Future improvements in sentiment and mood analysis, and ground truth obtained from user surveys, may increase the reliability of our SWB estimates. Second, given the large role that social media plays in the social lives of billions of individuals, we speculate that these environments may induce longitudinal changes in the public social behavior and may over time alter the very nature of social relations themselves []. Further analysis will be required to determine the extent and significance of these changes, and how they affect the propensity of online users to experience the effects of a Friendship and Happiness Paradox over time. Lastly, we stress that our study is, by definition, observational in nature. We do not and can not make any claims with respect to the causal nature of the relationships between Subjective Well-Being, popularity, mood contagion, and social network use. This will require an extensive program of experimental verification over extended periods of time which we are planning. Funding Bruno Gonçalves thanks the Moore and Sloan Foundations for support as part of the Moore-Sloan Data Science Environment at NYU. Johan Bollen thanks the Defense Advance Research Projects Agency (NGS2 2016 #D17AC00005) and the National Science Foundation (SMA-SBE: 1636636) for their support. Authors’ contributions JB designed and executed the experimental methodology, performed the data analysis, and wrote the manuscript. IL performed data analysis, advised on methodological and analytical issues, and co-authored the manuscript. BG collected the data, advised on methodological and analytical issues, and co-authored the manuscript. GR collected and processed the raw data. All authors reads and approved the final manuscript. 1. Apicella CL , Marlowe FW , Fowler JH , Christakis NA ( 2012 ) Social networks and cooperation in hunter-gatherers . Nature 481 : 497 - 501 2. Dunbar R ( 2010 ) The social role of touch in humans and primates: behavioural function and neurobiological mechanisms . Neurosci Biobehav Rev 34 ( 2 ): 260 - 268 3. Steptoe A , Shankar A , Demakakos P , Wardle J ( 2013 ) Social isolation, loneliness, and all-cause mortality in older men and women . Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 110 : 5797 - 5801 4. Pinker S ( 2015 ) The village effect: why face-to-face contact matters . Atlantic Books , London 5. Perrin A ( 2015 ) Social media usage: 2005-2015 . Report, Pew Research Center 6. Burke M , Marlow C , Lento T ( 2010 ) Social network activity and social well-being . In: Proceedings of the SIGCHI conference on human factors in computing systems , CHI '10. ACM , New York, pp 1909 - 1912 7. Kross E , Verduyn P , Demiralp E , Park J, Lee DS , Lin N , Shablack H , Jonides J , Ybarra O ( 2013 ) Facebook use predicts declines in subjective well-being in young adults . PLoS ONE 8 ( 8 ): e69841 8. Tromholt M , Lundby M , Andsbjerg K , Wiking M ( 2015 ) The Facebook experiment: does social media affect the quality of our lives ? Technical report, The Happiness Institute 9. Mitchell L , Frank MR , Decker Harris K , Dodds PS , Danforth CM ( 2013 ) The geography of happiness: connecting Twitter sentiment and expression, demographics, and objective characteristics of place . PLoS ONE 8 ( 5 ): e64417 10. Lin L , Sidani JE , Shensa A , Radovic A , Miller E , Colditz JB , Hoffman BL , Giles LM , Primack BA ( 2016 ) Association between social media use and depression among U.S. young adults. Depress. Anxiety 33 ( 4 ): 323 - 331 11. Feld SL ( 1991 ) Why your friends have more friends than you do . Am J Sociol 96 : 1464 - 1477 12. Hodas NO , Kooti F , Lerman K ( 2013 ) Friendship paradox redux: your friends are more interesting than you . In: Proceedings of the seventh international AAAI conference on weblogs and social media . AAAI Press, Menlo Park 13. Oswald AJ , Wu S ( 2010 ) Objective confirmation of subjective measures of human well-being: evidence from the U .S.A. Science 327 ( 5965 ): 576 - 579 14. Krueger AB , Stone AA ( 2014 ) Progress in measuring subjective well-being . Science 346 ( 6205 ): 42 - 43 15. Posel DR , Casale DM ( 2011 ) Relative standing and subjective well-being in South Africa: the role of perceptions, expectations and income mobility . Soc Indic Res 104 : 195 - 223 16. de la Garza A , Mastrobuoni G , Sannabe A , Yamada K ( 2010 ) The relative utility hypothesis with and without self-reported reference wages . Carlo Alberto Notebooks 159 , Collegio Carlo Alberto 17. Bollen J , Gonçalves B , Ruan G , Mao H ( 2011 ) Happiness is assortative in online social networks . Artif Life 17 : 237 - 251 18. Gonçalves B , Perra N , Vespignani A ( 2011 ) Modeling users' activity on Twitter: validation of Dunbar's number . PLoS ONE 6:e22656 19. Wilson T , Hoffmann P , Somasundaran S , Kessler J , Wiebe J , Choi Y , Cardie C , Riloff E , Patwardhan S ( 2005 ) Opinionfinder: a system for subjectivity analysis . In: Conference on empirical methods in natural language processing , pp 34 - 35 20. Ribeiro FN , Araújo M , Gonçalves P , Gonçalves MA , Benevenuto F ( 2016 ) Sentibench - a benchmark comparison of state-of-the-practice sentiment analysis methods . EPJ Data Sci 5 ( 1 ): 23 21. Stiller B , Bocek T , Hecht F , Machado G , Racz P , Waldburger M ( 2010 ) Measuring national well-being: proposed well-being indicators . Technical report, The Commission on Measuring Well-being, Japan 22. McPherson M , Smith-Lovin L , Cook JM ( 2001 ) Birds of a feather: homophily in social networks . Annu Rev Sociol 27 ( 1 ): 415 - 444 23. Fowler JH , Christakis NA ( 2008 ) Dynamic spread of happiness in a large social network: longitudinal analysis over 20 years in the Framingham heart study . Br Med J 337:a2338 24. Kramer ADI , Guillory JE , Hancock JT ( 2014 ) Experimental evidence of massive-scale emotional contagion through social networks . Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 111 : 8788 - 8790 25. Ferrara E , Yang Z ( 2015 ) Measuring emotional contagion in social media . PLoS ONE 10:e0142390 26. Ferrara E , Yang Z ( 2015 ) Quantifying the effect of sentiment on information diffusion in social media . PeerJ Comput Sci 1:e26 27. Dunbar R ( 2012 ) Social cognition on the Internet: testing constraints on social network size . Philos Trans R Soc Lond B, Biol Sci 367 ( 1599 ): 2192 - 2201


This is a preview of a remote PDF: https://link.springer.com/content/pdf/10.1140%2Fepjds%2Fs13688-017-0100-1.pdf

The happiness paradox: your friends are happier than you, EPJ Data Science, 2017, DOI: 10.1140/epjds/s13688-017-0100-1