Counts of mechanical, external configurations compared to computational, internal configurations in natural and artificial systems

PLOS ONE, May 2019

Animal movement encodes information that is meaningfully interpreted by natural counterparts. This is a behavior that roboticists are trying to replicate in artificial systems but that is not well understood even in natural systems. This paper presents a count on the cardinality of a discretized posture space—an aspect of expressivity—of articulated platforms. The paper uses an information-theoretic measure, Shannon entropy, to create observations analogous to Moore’s Law, providing a measure that complements traditional measures of the capacity of robots. This analysis, applied to a variety of natural and artificial systems, shows trends in increasing capacity in both internal and external complexity for natural systems while artificial, robotic systems have increased significantly in the capacity of computational (internal) states but remained more or less constant in mechanical (external) state capacity. The quantitative measure proposed in this paper provides an additional lens through which to compare natural and artificial systems.

A PDF file should load here. If you do not see its contents the file may be temporarily unavailable at the journal website or you do not have a PDF plug-in installed and enabled in your browser.

Alternatively, you can download the file locally and open with any standalone PDF reader:

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/article/file?id=10.1371/journal.pone.0215671&type=printable

Counts of mechanical, external configurations compared to computational, internal configurations in natural and artificial systems

May Counts of mechanical, external configurations compared to computational, internal configurations in natural and artificial systems Mechanical Science 0 Engineering 0 University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign 0 Urbana 0 United States of America 0 0 Editor: Denis Horvath, Pavol Jozef Safarik University in Kosice , SLOVAKIA Animal movement encodes information that is meaningfully interpreted by natural counterparts. This is a behavior that roboticists are trying to replicate in artificial systems but that is not well understood even in natural systems. This paper presents a count on the cardinality of a discretized posture space-an aspect of expressivity-of articulated platforms. The paper uses an information-theoretic measure, Shannon entropy, to create observations analogous to Moore's Law, providing a measure that complements traditional measures of the capacity of robots. This analysis, applied to a variety of natural and artificial systems, shows trends in increasing capacity in both internal and external complexity for natural systems while artificial, robotic systems have increased significantly in the capacity of computational (internal) states but remained more or less constant in mechanical (external) state capacity. The quantitative measure proposed in this paper provides an additional lens through which to compare natural and artificial systems. - Funding: A.L. received award #D16AP00001 from the Defense Advanced Research Projects Agency (DARPA, www.darpa.mil). The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript. Competing interests: I have read the journal?s policy and the author of this manuscript has the Introduction Moving bodies seem to express information: animals, including humans, communicate through non-verbal, visual cues. For example, a nervous twitch of the eye, slight straightening of the spine, or subtle quickening of breath of a poker player may involuntarily occur because they have just drawn a fortuitous card, reflecting a change of their internal estimate of their likelihood to win the hand. Another player may, having watched this opponent over the course of several hands, recognize this ?tell? and adjust their own strategy accordingly. Or, perhaps the first player is bluffing, and the action of the eye, spine, and breath are created to throw off their opponents. In either case, information is being transmitted and received. Inside the context of a poker game, these changes in external presentation have meaning. One small part of the example described in the previous paragraph is the capacity for humans to transmit information through their bodies. Motion over time can transmit arbitrary amounts of information (consider an LED using Morse Code to encode information), but the expressiveness of instantaneous postures may be an important practical measure of information sharing (consider an array of LEDs versus the single LED). Such a measure would not quantify information obtained from velocity of change in posture, which is an important potential source of information. Moreover, any metric on source complexity does not capture the effect of a source on a receiver in context (consider an array of LEDs displaying information in a language unfamiliar to the human receiver). This paper uses a traditional, information-theoretic communication model, relating each element of the example to either a source (the player who draws a card), channel (the poker game), or receiver (the other players at the table) [ 1 ]. Inside this picture, this paper deals only with establishing order-of-magnitude bounds on the practical capacity of a mechanical system as an information source. Prior work has used approximate entropy as a measure of system complexity [ 2 ], and more recently, symplectic principle component analysis and Shannon entropy have been used to characterized complex systems, such as time series data of electroencephalograms of human brains and vibrations from mechanical systems [3]. This paper will take a bottom-up modeling approach, counting the number of configurations available to systems and using Shannon entropy to measure posture spaces that are discretized according to the resolution of sensing architecture. Animal posture and movement encode information that is meaningfully interpreted by natural counterparts [ 4 ]. Coordination in groups [ 5 ], social interactions [6], and cuing during physical collaboration [ 7 ] are tasks that autonomous agents are being designed to replace or supplement natural counterparts where information about internal state of the system needs to be expressed through a physical body. Fish schooling [ 8, 9 ] and starling flocks [10] (to name a few) have been studied through the lens of dynamical systems theory and related to human crowd behavior [ 11 ]; in each case, agents take local cues from nearby neighbors to produce complex global behavior. Similarly, in dyadic interactions between humans, mirroring movements facilitate communication transfer [ 12 ]. These are examples where phenomena modeled by continuous dynamic equations facilitate motion coordination. But, at the same time, discrete information transfer is occurring. Evidence shows that behavior complexity increases with encephalization quotient across a sampling of species [ 13 ], and this type of trend analysis by species has been shown to correlate number of distinct behaviors observed with number of distinct muscle types [ 14 ]. Another way of asking these questions is through lens of imitation. The success of this goal is often determined through a functional lens, where one aspect of behavior of a system component, e.g., a specific joint angle behavior over time, is faithfully recreated. The review in [ 15 ] discusses ?robots that imitate humans?, describing ?very high fidelity playback.? This notion of imitation refers to matching the value of specific joint angles of each system over time. This established route of imitation through trajectory-based comparison of data capture is used by many researchers, comparing to human counterparts [ 16 ] and other animals [ 17?19 ], with particular attention to the precision of end points of appendages [20] or rate of activity [21]. In [22?27] motion capture data, a sparse representation, using a 10s or 100s of degrees of freedom, of human movement, seeds artificial system behavior. In this vein, even very simple (2 degree-of freedom) robots have been described by humans as ?imitating? human motion capture data when shown correlated behavior over time [28] On the other hand, models in animation are orders of magnitude more complex, using 1,000s and 10,000s of parameters [29?31]. That is, researchers do not have a clear agreement about the order of magnitude of the number of distinct postures available to humans or whether that complexity aids agility. Moreover, features measured through complexity may better align with the trends identified in the uncanny effect of some human-like machines [32]. Optimization has been proposed to describe natural motion for decades. The minimum jerk model [33] has been extended over time with many variants [34], including one that leverages stochasticity [35]. In that work, Todorov provides a model that reconciles human 2 / 19 complexity with human performance in complex environments, arguing that humans have many more degrees of freedom than they need but that this makes humans robust to failures in dynamic environments [36]. In factory settings, robots have been out-performing humans for decades, offering greater precision, higher-payloads, and consistency in repeatable tasks [37?39]. Imitating the movement of biological organisms, using similar, trajectory-matching metrics for success, has been a topic in animation [40] and robotics [41, 42]. From the point of view of Newton and mechanics, robots are superior mechanical devices. Yet, despite an explosion of computing power, including cloud-based devices, robots do not thrive in dynamic environments nor can they recreate the social behaviors of humans, even under teleoperation [ 43 ]. In other words, by the continuous, mechanical models of dynamical systems where the integral relationships between position, velocity, and force are demonstrated, robots are outperforming humans, but in practice robots are not rapidly replacing humans in complex environments. The idea that communication through movement may require behavior that counters efficiency has been demonstrated through the comparison of two defined ideas ?predictability? and ?legibility? in [ 44 ]. This work provides a clear model for how efficiency and clarity of intent may be at odds, but considers only simple goal transmission through movement, distinguishing between two objects: this bottle or that. In poker, there are 10 different categories of hands, e.g., a flush, a full house, with about 2.5 million hands possible. In other words, there is likely more bandwidth than one bit in the channel between humans engaged in this activity. Previous work has explored how much information is needed in tasks of sensing the environment for artificial systems [ 45, 46 ] and natural ones [47]. Many of these same trajectory-based, continuous joint angle measures have been applied in social contexts where varied messages are created through modulation of movement design. Human actors provide motion trajectories for ?expressive? or ?affective? movement in mobile robotics [ 48 ], articulated robots [ 49 ], and gait animation [ 50 ]. These efforts, which showcase the power of work by artists, show it?s possible to communicate through artificial systems. However, the capacity to communicate through machines is noticeably limited. Indeed, in successful examples, like movies, performances, and other artistic work, the context around these machines (real or simulated) must be highly controlled. Prior work shows that even a motion model derived from human motion (and not as yet implementable on machines) expresses very different ideas when exposed to and situated in variable environmental contexts [ 51 ]. It?s been shown that people read narrative even into only ?apparent? behavior [ 52 ]. But, in practice work, artists have found that this is especially true for robotic motion [ 53 ], which has, in some way, less specificity than that of human counterparts. This paper provides a quantitative measure that may explain this observation and may help understand a key difference between natural and current artificial systems. Materials and methods To begin, consider some established examples of information sources: ? A simple LED. It is always in one of 2 states. By flashing on and off, it can communicate one bit of information. Using a coding scheme, e.g., Morse code, this system can transmit an arbitrary amount of information given an infinite time horizon. ? A trio of three LEDs. It is always in one of 23 = 8 states. By flashing each LED on and off in turn, it can communicate three bits of information. We might say that adding two LEDs to the previous system created a more expressive system. 3 / 19 ? A large array of n LEDs. It is at any given time in one of 2n states. By turning on and off elements in this array, more complex messages, of size n bits, can be transmitted. Given an infinite time window, these systems can all communicate arbitrary amounts of information. What distinguishes their level of practical usefulness is the number of externally visible configurations accessible to the system. Unsurprisingly, computer monitors are essentially comprised of many lighted elements, roughly equivalent to LEDs, whereas the on/off indicator on a coffee pot may only be a single LED. This reflects the fact that the complexity of the internal state that each of these systems need to communicate to the environment is different. The discrete measure listed above, quantifying the capacity of LED sources, has limitations: ? Potential correlations in on/off patterns may result in compressibility of the source behavior, e.g., if all LEDs fire on together all of the time a sparser representation is possible. ? Dynamic changes, e.g., a notably slow versus quick onset of the ?on? state, can also add to source complexity and are not captured. When considering information transfer to humans, important limitations of these receivers should be enumerated, as they come into play for mechanical systems too: ? The timescale of the source changes may be too fast for the human to perceive. ? The brightness of the on state may not offer enough contrast for the human to perceive. ? The size of the individual LEDs may be too small for the human to perceive. ? The human receiver may not know Morse code (or whichever encoding scheme is used). Note that these limitations are not homogeneously distributed in the population. Some humans know Morse Code; some do not. Some have more sensitive eyes than others. Moreover, if the culture or upbringing of the human receiver is different from the one who encoded the message in the first place, the meaning of the message may be lost. There are plenty of schemes, like grouping the action of LEDs to provide the correct resolution or using an intuitive encoding that will improve the performance of the system in transmitting information across a channel to a receiver. Thus, to quantify the complexity of this proposed source, let N be the number of actuator types on a machine. Let Mi be the number of degrees of freedom with a particular number of available configurations Ri, which is computed via counting from an actuators minimum to maximum range via its resolution. From this description of a machine?s construction, is the number of discrete, kinematic configurations (shapes) available to a platform. From there one measure of the capacity of that platform is the amount of information, or Shannon entropy, which is measured in bits, needed to uniquely identify a configuration for that platform. This is given by C ? Y N RiMi i?1 K ? log 2?C?: ?1? ?2? On an array of n LEDs (or n transistors), this equation becomes K ? log 2? 2n? ? n: Y 1 i?1 ?3? ?4? ?5? On a robot with a simple gripper (which is either open or closed), two identical servos, and a single LED N = 3. Assume each servo has 360? range and is sensed by an encoder with 0.1? resolution with a gripper that may be ?open? or ?closed? and an LED that may be ?on? or ?off?, R1 = 3600 with M1 = 2 and R2 = 2 with M2 = 2. This becomes Thus, this simplistic robot can be compared to a 25-bit display in terms of the cardinality of a discretized space of possible mechanical configurations, which is limited in a meaningful way by the resolution of sensing, actuation, and control subsystems. Any computer that is Turing-complete can, in theory, run the same programs as other complete machines. However, the number of transistors in the CPU is a useful, implementationspecific measure (which has been growing) to understand how practically powerful a given machine is, despite the static nature of this counting. After two detailed examples are presented, a similar analysis will quantify patterns observed through using robots in dance performances mentioned in the Introduction. Example: SoftBank/Aldebaran NAO humanoid robot Example: Bellagio water fountains Consider a tourist attraction, like the Bellagio water fountains in Las Vegas, NV. Tourists line up every hour to watch this famous display, routinely included in lists of popular Vegas attractions. This is to say that the fountain display is visually very interesting for human watchers; therefore, it should be more expressive than the NAO robot. The fountain has about 1,200 water cannons with 5,000 lights as part of its display. It also has the ability to create fog and features popular music during the shows. For this analysis [ 58, 59 ], consider only the water cannons and lights. The cannons come in four types: robotic Oarsman and three sizes of Shooters. The 208 Oarsman are articulated cannons with active control; the Shooters simply blast water at three predetermined pressure settings, each having a single pressure setting according to their size. The lights can be a range of colors. Table 2 articulates a model for this system. For the Oarsman, which rotate about two axes, assume a range of motion of 160? with a resolution of 1? in each dimension. Assume the water shooting out of the cannon to be on or off with a single pressure setting. Likewise, the Shooters, are either on or off without articulation. The lights can be ?off? or one of twelve colors (as modeled by a moderate segmentation of the color wheel). The following analysis will ignore the music that plays alongside. 6 / 19 Computing the instantaneous mechanical complexity, ignoring the lights, we find the following: Thus, the proposed metric provides one possible quantitative bound on how much more expressive the fountains are than the prior humanoid example. In this case, about two orders of magnitude with respect to the amount of information they can encode. This result is consistent with the considerably great system expense and tourist attendance of the fountains; however, any analysis of the physical characteristics of either system cannot explain the interest on the part of humans in either system. The city of Las Vegas, inclusion in movies, and other cultural factors play a large role in the context surrounding these expressive fountains. Music and the sound of water spray, which also accompanies the fountain displays, also adds an audible degree of freedom while the feel of rushing air adds a haptic channel that adds to the experience. On the other hand, it may be that the human-like form of the NAO robot makes it particularly interesting to human observers. This measure merely quantifies one small aspect of the myriad of factors that create entertainment value and meaningful motion for human observers. Moreover, as in the previous example, we do not capture the additional expressivity that the dynamics of timing and water add to (and take away from) the system. For example, by moving at variable speeds, these fountains can create different patterns in the water, which add to the system?s complexity. On the other hand, in the presence of water, not all points in the cannon?s range might be physically feasible. What if all the water cannons were the articulated, Oarsman variety? In that case, we have: Sensitivity analysis In this analysis Mi (note ?i Mi = N) and Ri are platform-specific parameters that may involve estimation, particularly for natural systems, and are sources for model error. This section considers the sensitivity of the model to two sources of uncertainty: number of degrees of freedom and precision of each degree of freedom. Let ?i be the uncertainty associated with the number of degrees of freedom of a particular type, Mi. If the number is too low, ?i should be added to Mi and if Mi is an overestimate, ?i should be subtracted. Let i be the uncertainty associated with the precision of Ri. If the precision used to generate Ri is too coarse, then this factor needs to divide Ri while if it is too fine, then this factor needs to multiply Ri. These parameters can be 7 / 19 incorporated into the calculation, giving a bound on the accuracy of the true entropy K: log 2? Y N i?1 Ri= iMi Zi ? K log 2? Ri iMi?Zi ? Y N i?1 where i 1 and ?i 0 and if i = 1 and ?i = 0, this corresponds to zero uncertainty, returning Eq 3. To check the sensitivity of this model to each source of uncertainty note the following algebraic expansion of the upper bound on K: For the lower bound, we have: i?1 ZN log2 RN X N i?1 K M1 log2 R1 ? Z1 log2 R1 ? M1 log2 1 ? Z1 log2 1 ? . . . . . . ? MN log2 RN ? ZN log2 RN ? MN log2 N ? ZN log2 N X N ) K K ? ?Zi?log2 Ri ? log2 i? ? Mi?log2 i? : K M1 log2 R1 Z1 log2 R1 M1 log2 1= 1 Z1 log2 1 ? . . . . . . ? MN log2 RN MN log2 1= N ZN log2 N ) K K ?Zi?log2 Ri ? log2 i? Mi?log2 i? : Thus, error on K scales linearly with ?i (degrees of freedom) and logarithmically with i (precision). Limitations of the proposed measure As with the LED examples previously discussed, the measure proposed here has limitations: ? Potential correlations in exhibited motion may result in compressibility of the source behavior. ? Dynamic changes can also add to source complexity and are not captured. Moreover, we can acknowledge a similar set of limitations when considering information transfer to humans: ? The timescale of the mechanical changes may be too fast for the human to perceive. ? The change in kinematic position may not offer enough contrast for the human to perceive. ? The size of the changing mechanism may be too small for the human to perceive. ? The human receiver may not know the encoding scheme used by the source, in particular, anthropomorphic systems may have an advantage; this point includes cultural context (which we could consider a type of encoding scheme) as described in the case of the famous Las Vegas landmark, the Bellagio Fountains. The proposed measure categorizes the instantaneous complexity of a mechanical source, as has been useful in communication theory, ignoring the issues of the channel and receiver. For example, some contextual situations require very simple messages for meaningful interaction, say, answering ?this or that? or ?yes or no?. Further, human-imitating systems may be more effective in communication to humans. Nor should systems that communicate with humans 8 / 19 ?10? ?11? ?12? need to be limited to human shapes?as there are many examples in nature and animated films where non-anthropomorphic characters are quite expressive: we communicate well with dogs and characters of many fictitious forms, for which encoding schemes are learned through contextual interaction and narrative. Results and discussion The measure and procedure presented in the previous section can categorize and compare artificial systems. The results of applying this measure to a variety of robotic platforms and natural systems is presented here. Limitations and assumptions in the analysis are presented as well as discussion of trends in the data. Analysis of artificial systems The plot in Fig 1 shows the number of transistors in the onboard CPU of a range of robots over the past 15 years. While, as many platforms are now Internet connected, it is a limiting picture of the computational power available to these platforms, plots like this have been used to track the progress of computational power over time, which has roughly doubled every year, even serving as a driving goal for the industry (Moore?s Law [ 60 ]). In such plots each additional component on an integrated circuit represents the ability to represent a larger?or Fig 1. Internal complexity in artificial systems over time. The number of transistors, t, used in onboard processors of robots over the last fifteen years. The number of internal configurations available is then 2t. Some platform names are omitted for clarity; see S1 Appendix for full list. 9 / 19 Fig 2. External complexity in artificial systems over time. The number of external configurations available, C, for mechanization in robot platforms over time. Several platforms have an assumed positioning resolution of 0.1? and an estimated range of motion. Some platform names are omitted for clarity; see S1 Appendix for full list. more precise?number on a single chip. Each new transistor adds a new power of 2 in representation precision. Note, that the the number of transistors in modern, stand-alone processors is in the billions. To convert that to possible machine configurations, where the actuators are transistors, the number 2 (which is the number of configurations for each actuator) has to be raised to that large number, resulting in a number of configurations that is bounded by 21011 or 1030102999566. This number is an important, practical measure of how many numbers the computers onboard robots can represent?or how expressive these internal systems are. Processor speeds and operating system design govern the dynamics of these systems and whether this baseline capacity is used well?counting transistors does not account for this. Fig 2 shows how this measure has evolved on the mechanical configurations of robots over time. The plot shows the number of possible kinematic configurations, C, for a number of rigid-body robots whose motion is governed by motors and encoders measuring their position over time. The degrees of freedom of each robot can be estimated from publications, videos, and online reports; in several cases, the precision of onboard sensors, e.g., rotary encoders in joints, has been assumed to be 0.1 units as in [ 57, 61, 62 ] (see S1 Appendix for further discussion and full list of parameters). According to the estimates used here, the corresponding practical measure of how externally complex these robots are is bounded by 10140. Like Moore?s 10 / 19 Fig 3. Internal Vs. external complexity in artificial systems over time. A comparison of computational complexity (# of transistors in the CPU) relative to a measure of mechanical complexity (K) on robotic platforms over time. Platform names are omitted for clarity; see S1 Appendix for full list. The unit of measure on both axes is bits; square plot axes highlight imbalance between internal and external states (although note that the axes use different ranges). proxy of the number of transistors within a single chip, this discretized configuration space does not account for dynamics but gives a starting point for measurement and comparison of instantaneous postural complexity. By converting the number of configurations C to a number represented in a base 2 number system, K, the change in the computational capacities of these platforms can be compared to their mechanization capacities as shown in Fig 3. This log-log plot provides a comparison in terms of the number of bits which it would take to describe the largest number that would fit in the onboard CPU versus the number of bits needed to uniquely represent each pose. The plot reveals an explosion of internal complexity and a flat trend in the mechanical, external complexity over the past 15 years. The complexity analysis used in the plot shows an imbalance between computation and mechanization capacities. Through this lens, the external complexity of the NAO, the small humanoid robot examined in the prior section, is comparable to the internal complexity a 1960s computer chip with only 256 transistors. This analysis suggests that there could be a magnitude of external complexity that going beyond does not improve system performance or produces intractably complex systems. The next section will attempt to look at natural systems to test this idea further. 11 / 19 Analysis of natural systems The previous section outlines a way to compare robot capacity for complex behavior, but the same method can be applied to biological creatures?with the caveat that such systems may involve processes not extant in today?s machines. For example, the difference between sensing and actuation blurs for these systems where movement is tightly linked to sensing, e.g. fovea, and complex, compliant materials comprise much of these systems, e.g., cartilage. However, we can loosely compare ?computation? to internal state changes, which do not meaningfully impact the environment, and ?mechanization? to external state changes, which cause direct change in the environment. In prior work, the movement of a microscopic worm (C. Elegans) was analyzed using a curve parameterized by 100 angles; then, after capturing an extended period of behavior on laboratory agar, a principle component analysis was performed, revealing that the structure of the behavior could be explained as a superposition of four primary poses [ 63 ]. Observing the animal through this lens provided new insights into the behavior of this well-studied animal [ 64, 65 ], implying a meaningful parameterization, further explored in [66]. Stephen?s model can be used to compare the C. Elegans to modern robots. A C. Elegans has 302 neurons, which can be approximated to be either ?firing? or ?not? in a static snapshot of time. (This is a commonly used under-approximation, for neurons, which are now believed to use graded, rather than binary, firing mechanisms [ 67 ].) Then, each of the 100 angles have a typical range, shown in the empirical results in [ 63 ], between ?1.5 radians to 1.5 radians, implying 0.1 radians of precision. Thus, using the metric proposed in Eq 3, the kinematic mechanization capacity for this model of a simple animal can be calculated as K ? log 2?30100? ? log 2?5:2 10147 configurations? 491 bits: ?13? The linear model derived in [ 63 ] can also be used to derive a slightly more compact expression, finding a ?compression? that removes correlations in joint angles exhibited. The idea is that the organism, in a particular task or environment, does not utilize the full span of motion modeled by 100 angles. This could be a function of behavioral patterning, environment, or experimental set up; however, the larger behavioral space intoned by the anatomical model may be used in future, unforeseen environments. Here, linear combinations of four ?eigenworms? describe 95% of the observed worm behavior. Follow on work estimates this number to be six [ 66 ], but the original four-dimensional model is used here. As an aside, given these lower-dimensional models, it may be easy to then discount the more complex model of behavior entirely, but consider the behavior shown in pavement ants taken to the International Space Station [ 68 ]. These ants exhibited unseen behavior for their species in microgravity, suggesting that in new environments animals express new behaviors. In the model proposed here, we suggest that postures exhibited in a particular environment are a compression of possible postures. Namely, without a rich enough original high-dimensional space, the lower-dimensional model could not be identified. Thus, both the rich, higherdimensional model and the sparse, lower-dimensional model derived from observed data will be considered in the analysis to follow. The weights on each eigenworm, ?1, ?2, ?3, and ?4, also have an observed range, which can be estimated from [ 63 ] as ?1: [ ?2, 2 ], ?2: [ ?2, 2 ], and ?3: [ ?5, 5 ]. Four the fourth, which was not found to have behavioral significance and is not provided in [63], assume ?4: [ ?2, 2 ]. For each, assume 0.1 in precision based on reported significant figures in [63]. This leads to the 12 / 19 Fig 4. Complexity of natural and artificial systems. The machine data points, plotted in black stars, are the same as those in Figs 1?3. Corresponding points for two models of C. Elegans are plotted in black circles. These x-axis points are based on the number of neurons (302); the y-axis data points reflect established behavioral models, one based on anatomy and the other based on exhibited behavior on agar [ 63 ]. Other organisms, with less established behavioral models, are plotted in blue circles. The x-axis values for the other organisms are also based on the estimated number of neurons [ 69 ]; the y-axis values are given by: Drosophila-rough-anatomy: analysis of [ 70 ]; Cat-muscles: generalizing the model of muscles in [ 71 ]; Humans-WAeval: the structure of [ 72 ]; Humans-mocap: the structure of a Natural Point OptiTrack motion capture file; and Humans-breath: the simulation in [29]. See S1 Appendix for data detail. following calculation: K ? log 2?403 1001? ? log 2?6:4 106 configurations? 23 bits: ?14? The summary of this analysis is plotted in black in Fig 4. The plot gives a sense of the capacity of today?s robots. It is shown that, through the lens proposed here, C. Elegans are apt natural analogs for the mechanical complexity of many robots. While, clearly, C. Elegans cannot replicate any of the specific functions that robots accomplish, perhaps they can do equivalently complex tasks. That is, for example, painting a particular, single part over and over might also be accomplished by a linear combination of four postures. This is consistent with the fact that today?s robots are created for single-tasks in controlled environments. C. Elegans must forage for food and carry out other critical to life tasks in real, dynamic environments. Note that 13 / 19 additional factors need also to be considered in this comparison. For example, there are distinct Reynolds numbers at play. Inertial forces dominate the selected robots, while viscous forces dominate the motion of C. Elegans; however, this analysis is not considering forces, only the complexity of postural snapshots. Such detailed motion models for other animals are not common, but a few estimates and examples are included in blue in Fig 4. Relevant work done on fruit flies (Drosophila) abstracts their state as a vector with a heading over time [ 73 ], but an anatomical analysis via [ 70 ] can estimate postural complexity of the organism. Researchers look to muscle activity involved in postural control for cats with weights having precision to the hundredth decimal place [ 71 ]; generalizing this precision to all estimated 517 [ 74 ] muscles in cats gives an estimate of the complexity of their muscular control, leaving out the mechanical advantages of their compliant bones, skin, and hair. For human motion, three different established guides to behavior (a public health diagnostic [ 72 ], a Natural Point OptiTrak motion capture file, and an animation of breathing [29]) of body posture can be used to show similarity between these models and those that identified behavioral understanding of C. Elegans. The balance between internal and external capacity seen in C. Elegans is not found in robots or models of other organisms. This could be due to a lack of benefit in increased external complexity, suggesting that there may be a natural threshold on order-of-magnitude complexity, around which systems cluster. However, the discrepancy between human motion models suggest that we may not yet appreciate the role of complexity in natural motion (and whether it is needed to describe the Shannon entropy of these systems). Conclusion The paper has introduced a measure for postural expressivity, clarifying prior points of view on function versus expression in movement by relating these two ideas in the same measure. Finally, the paper uses this proposed measure to compare extant artificial systems to natural systems, suggesting that, through the lens of complexity, many modern robots, including humanoids, are comparable to a microscopic worm. This provides a quantitative model for ways in which robots still have to improve to recreate the behavior of natural counterparts (without negating the fact that the force and velocity profiles of these machines can certainly exceed that of C. Elegans and many natural systems). Future work will extend the discretized configuration space presented here to a discretized state space, according to actuator force and torque limits, in order to account for information transmitted through variable velocity. Discrete measures may be important in highlighting the differences between natural and artificial systems. For example, an Ising model has been used to describe differences between natural and human-made, urban environments where images of natural scenes versus urban ones are shown to have different statistical properties [ 75 ]. To create more characters in a font style, such as emoji, a more complex underlying ASCII representation is needed [ 76 ]. This paper uses a similar approach to begin to characterize the ability for systems to transmit information through their mechanical motion. This may begin to bring to bear a notion of expressivity in robotics that is consistent with usage in computer science [ 77 ], genetics [ 78 ], psychology [ 79 ], and dance. Moreover, this measure may aid in comparisons of soft-bodied, color-changing animals with many appendages like some cephalopods to mammals like baboons, which are discussed in [ 80 ] in terms of the expressivity of their behavior. This work provides evidence relevant to the discussion around the effect of machine-based automation of human tasks, particularly in manufacturing. Humans operate in dynamic environments where variability in movement is essential in accommodating and coping with an unpredictable world. Robots do well?and in many cases are superior?in repeatable tasks 14 / 19 where environmental factors are controlled (as in a factory), but do not have the same capacity to adapt or function in complex, variable environments. The trends pointed to here in robotics may help guide system development: in hardware, soft and compliant robots which inherently have more instantaneous complexity due to the rich space of shapes taken on by these systems, and, in software, increasing motion variability will offer robotic systems greater expressivity. In human-robot interaction, quantitative guides on how many possible variable states a given platform can produce may help ensure clarity in user perception. Active, promising work in these areas include compliant mechanisms [ 81 ], soft components [ 82 ], and modular systems [ 83 ]. Further, considering the wide, possibly unused in typical activities, capacity for motion of natural systems, may produce improved understanding of how animals adapt. Just as information has been used to characterize the sensory systems of animals [ 84 ], it may prove to be a useful lens through which to study motor behavior. Supporting information S1 Appendix. Numbers used to calculate data points in Figs 1?3, including those in Tables 1 and 2, are provided as a download-able dataset. A few notes about the elements of this dataset are provided here: ? The list of 20 artificial systems analyzed is Kismet (2000), ASIMO (2000), PackBot (2001), Roomba (2002), RoboSapien (2003), BigDog (2005), KR 60 HA (2005), Aibo (2006), Keepon (2007), LittleDog (2007), NAO (2008), Simon (2009), Darwin (2010), PR2 (2010), RoboNaut2 (2010), Baxter (2012), Cheetah (2012), LBR iiwa (2013), HansonZeno (2013), and Khepera IV (2015). ? For the external element counts of artificial systems, many of these were determined through observation. The choice of 0.1 resolution, where not directly available, is supported by the use of this precision of encoders in the NAO platform [ 57 ], in a patent filed by FANUC Corp. for their industrial robots [ 61 ], and in a more recent patents on encoders by Bei Sensors and Systems Company, Inc. [ 62 ]. In these systems, encoders use optical sensors to detect light transmission through transparent regions on a circular disc; this analog signal is transmitted to digital representation, where the number of bits allotted can also impact resolution. A common number of transparencies is, as in the FANUC patent, 2500, and a common digital representation is 12 bits as in a more recent patent by Bei, allowing 4096 distinct positions. Across 360? both result in roughly 0.1? precision. In applications like laser jet printers, these devices can also achieve micrometer precision (roughly 1 million positions out of one rotation) as discussed in a patent from Lexmark International, Inc. [ 85 ]. To this author?s knowledge, such specialized, high-precision hardware is not common in the robots reviewed here. ? For the natural systems analyzed, the values used are based on [ 29, 63, 70?72, 74 ]. (XLSX) Acknowledgments Undergraduate students Jialu Li and Varun Jain helped collect and estimate data on each artificial platform reviewed. Drs. Lisa LaViers, Todd Murphey, and Joseph Rokicki provided insightful, helpful comments on earlier drafts. Author Contributions Conceptualization: Amy LaViers. 15 / 19 Funding acquisition: Amy LaViers. Investigation: Amy LaViers. Methodology: Amy LaViers. Supervision: Amy LaViers. Writing ? original draft: Amy LaViers. Writing ? review & editing: Amy LaViers. 16 / 19 20. Meijer J, Lei Q, Wisse M. Performance study of single-query motion planning for grasp execution using various manipulators. In: Advanced Robotics (ICAR), 2017 18th International Conference on. IEEE; 2017. p. 450?457. Hodson R. How robots are grasping the art of gripping. Nature. 2018; 557:S23?S25. https://doi.org/10. 1038/d41586-018-05093-1 PMID: 29743715 Yamane K, Kuffner JJ, Hodgins JK. Synthesizing animations of human manipulation tasks. In: ACM Transactions on Graphics (TOG). vol. 23. ACM; 2004. p. 532?539. 17 / 19 18 / 19 1. Cover TM , Thomas JA . Elements of information theory . John Wiley & Sons; 2012 . 2. Pincus SM . Approximate entropy as a measure of system complexity . Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences . 1991 ; 88 ( 6 ): 2297 - 2301 . https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.88.6. 2297 3. Lei M , Meng G , Zhang W , Wade J , Sarkar N. Symplectic Entropy as a novel measure for complex systems . Entropy . 2016 ; 18 ( 11 ): 412 . https://doi.org/10.3390/e18110412 4. Laidre ME , Johnstone RA . Animal signals . Current Biology . 2013 ; 23 ( 18 ): R829 - R833 . https://doi.org/ 10.1016/j.cub. 2013 . 07 .070 PMID: 24070440 5. Couzin ID , Krause J . Self-organization and collective behavior in vertebrates . Advances in the Study of Behavior . 2003 ; 32 . https://doi.org/10.1016/S0065- 3454 ( 03 ) 01001 - 5 6. Bakshy E , Rosenn I , Marlow C , Adamic L. The role of social networks in information diffusion . In: Proceedings of the 21st international conference on World Wide Web. ACM; 2012 . p. 519 - 528 . 7. Townsend EAMEC , Killpack MD . Analysis of Rigid Extended Object Co-Manipulation by Human Dyads: Lateral Movement Characterization . Robotics: Science and Systems (RSS) . 2017 ;. 8. Katz Y , Tunstr?m K , Ioannou CC , Huepe C , Couzin ID . Inferring the structure and dynamics of interactions in schooling fish . Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences . 2011 ; 108 ( 46 ): 18720 - 18725 . https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1107583108 9. Swain DT , Couzin ID , Leonard NE . Real-time feedback-controlled robotic fish for behavioral experiments with fish schools . Proceedings of the IEEE . 2012 ; 100 ( 1 ): 150 - 163 . https://doi.org/10.1109/ JPROC. 2011 .2165449 10. Young GF , Scardovi L , Cavagna A , Giardina I , Leonard NE . Starling flock networks manage uncertainty in consensus at low cost . PLoS computational biology . 2013 ; 9 ( 1 ):e1002894. https://doi.org/10.1371/ journal.pcbi. 1002894 PMID: 23382667 11. Dyer JR , Ioannou CC , Morrell LJ , Croft DP , Couzin ID , Waters DA , et al. Consensus decision making in human crowds . Animal Behaviour . 2008 ; 75 ( 2 ): 461 - 470 . https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav. 2007 . 05 . 010 12. Boker SM , Cohn JF , Theobald BJ , Matthews I , Brick TR , Spies JR . Effects of damping head movement and facial expression in dyadic conversation using real-time facial expression tracking and synthesized avatars . Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences . 2009 ; 364 ( 1535 ): 3485 - 3495 . https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb. 2009 .0152 PMID: 19884143 13. Changizi MA . Relationship between number of muscles, behavioral repertoire size, and encephalization in mammals . Journal of Theoretical Biology . 2003 ; 220 ( 2 ): 157 - 168 . https://doi.org/10.1006/jtbi. 2003 . 3125 PMID: 12468289 14. McMahon TA , Bonner JT . On size and life . Scientific American Library; 1983 . 15. Breazeal C , Scassellati B . Robots that imitate humans . Trends in cognitive sciences . 2002 ; 6 ( 11 ): 481 - 487 . https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364- 6613 ( 02 ) 02016 -8 PMID: 12457900 16. Ames AD . Human-inspired control of bipedal walking robots . IEEE Transactions on Automatic Control . 2014 ; 59 ( 5 ): 1115 - 1130 . https://doi.org/10.1109/TAC. 2014 .2299342 17. Hopkins JK , Spranklin BW , Gupta SK . A survey of snake-inspired robot designs . Bioinspiration & biomimetics . 2009 ; 4 ( 2 ): 021001 . https://doi.org/10.1088/ 1748 -3182/4/2/021001 18. Haldane DW , Peterson KC , Bermudez FLG , Fearing RS . Animal-inspired design and aerodynamic stabilization of a hexapedal millirobot . In: Robotics and Automation (ICRA) , 2013 IEEE International Conference on. IEEE; 2013 . p. 3279 - 3286 . 19. Cully A , Clune J , Tarapore D , Mouret JB . Robots that can adapt like animals . Nature . 2015 ; 521 ( 7553 ): 503 . https://doi.org/10.1038/nature14422 PMID: 26017452 Nakaoka S , Nakazawa A , Yokoi K , Ikeuchi K. Leg motion primitives for a dancing humanoid robot . In: Robotics and Automation , 2004. Proceedings. ICRA'04 . 2004 IEEE International Conference on. vol. 1 . IEEE; 2004 . p. 610 - 615 . Gillies M. Learning finite-state machine controllers from motion capture data . Computational Intelligence and AI in Games, IEEE Transactions on. 2009 ; 1 ( 1 ): 63 - 72 . https://doi.org/10.1109/TCIAIG. 2009 .2019630 Kulic D , Ott C , Lee D , Ishikawa J , Nakamura Y. Incremental learning of full body motion primitives and their sequencing through human motion observation . The International Journal of Robotics Research . 2012 ; 31 ( 3 ): 330 - 345 . https://doi.org/10.1177/0278364911426178 LaViers A , Egerstedt M . Style-based Abstractions for Human Motion Classification . ACM/IEEE 5th International Conference on Cyber-Physical Systems . 2014 ;. Joo H , Liu H , Tan L , Gui L , Nabbe B , Matthews I , et al. Panoptic studio: A massively multiview system for social motion capture . In: Proceedings of the IEEE International Conference on Computer Vision ; 2015 . p. 3334 - 3342 . Kaushik R , LaViers A. Imitating Human Movement Using a Measure of Verticality to Animate Low Degree-of-Freedom Non-humanoid Virtual Characters . In: International Conference on Social Robotics. Springer; 2018 . p. 588 - 598 . Zordan VB , Celly B , Chiu B , DiLorenzo PC . Breathe easy: model and control of simulated respiration for animation . In: Proceedings of the 2004 ACM SIGGRAPH/Eurographics symposium on Computer animation . Eurographics Association; 2004 . p. 29 - 37 . Sueda S , Kaufman A , Pai DK . Musculotendon simulation for hand animation . In: ACM Transactions on Graphics (TOG) . vol. 27 . ACM; 2008 . p. 83 . Sigal L , Balan AO , Black MJ . Humaneva: Synchronized video and motion capture dataset and baseline algorithm for evaluation of articulated human motion . International journal of computer vision . 2010 ; 87 ( 1-2 ):4. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11263-009-0273-6 Mori M , MacDorman KF , Kageki N. The uncanny valley [from the field] . IEEE Robotics & Automation Magazine . 2012 ; 19 ( 2 ): 98 - 100 . https://doi.org/10.1109/MRA. 2012 .2192811 Flash T , Hogan N. The coordination of arm movements: an experimentally confirmed mathematical model . Journal of neuroscience . 1985 ; 5 ( 7 ): 1688 - 1703 . https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.05-07- 01688 . 1985 PMID: 4020415 Srinivasan M , Ruina A . Computer optimization of a minimal biped model discovers walking and running . Nature . 2006 ; 439 ( 7072 ): 72 . https://doi.org/10.1038/nature04113 PMID: 16155564 Todorov E, Jordan MI . Optimal feedback control as a theory of motor coordination . Nature neuroscience . 2002 ; 5 ( 11 ): 1226 . https://doi.org/10.1038/nn963 PMID: 12404008 Valero-Cuevas FJ . Fundamentals of neuromechanics. Springer; 2016 . UR5 Technical specifications . Universal Robots; 2016 . Available from: https://www.universal-robots. com/media/50588/ur5_en. pdf [cited Accessed May 3 , 2018 ]. JACO2 6 DOF Advanced Specification Guide . Kinova Robotics; 2017 . Available from: http://www. kinovarobotics.com/wp-content/uploads/2015/02/JACO\begingroup\let\relax\relax\endgroup [Pleaseinsert\PrerenderUnicode{}intopreamble] -6DOF- Advanced- Specification-Guide. pdf [cited Accessed May 3 , 2018 ]. Sawyer Collaborative Robot Tech Specs. Rethink Robotics ; 2018 . Available from: http://www. rethinkrobotics.com/sawyer/tech-specs/ [cited Accessed May 3, 2018 ]. Reynolds CW . Steering behaviors for autonomous characters . In: Game developers conference . vol. 1999 ; 1999 . p. 763 - 782 . Egerstedt M , Balch T , Dellaert F , Delmotte F , Khan Z. What are the ants doing? vision-based tracking and reconstruction of control programs . In: Proceedings of the IEEE International Conference on Robotics and Automation (ICRA 2005 ) ; 2005 . p. 18 - 22 . Powell MJ , Zhao H , Ames AD . Motion primitives for human-inspired bipedal robotic locomotion: walking and stair climbing . In: Robotics and Automation (ICRA) , 2012 IEEE International Conference on. IEEE; 2012 . p. 543 - 549 . 43. Yanco HA , Norton A , Ober W , Shane D , Skinner A , Vice J . Analysis of human-robot interaction at the darpa robotics challenge trials . Journal of Field Robotics . 2015 ; 32 ( 3 ): 420 - 444 . https://doi.org/10.1002/ rob.21568 44. Dragan AD , Lee KC , Srinivasa SS . Legibility and predictability of robot motion . In: Human-Robot Interaction (HRI) , 2013 8th ACM/IEEE International Conference on. IEEE; 2013 . p. 301 - 308 . 45. Blum M , Kozen D . On the power of the compass (or, why mazes are easier to search than graphs) . In: Foundations of Computer Science , 1978 ., 19th Annual Symposium on . IEEE; 1978 . p. 132 - 142 . 46. Donald BR . On information invariants in robotics . Artificial Intelligence . 1995 ; 72 ( 1-2 ): 217 - 304 . https:// doi.org/10.1016/ 0004 - 3702 ( 94 ) 00024 -U 47. Palmer SE , Marre O , Berry MJ , Bialek W. Predictive information in a sensory population . Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences . 2015 ; 112 ( 22 ): 6908 - 6913 . https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1506855112 48. Knight H , Simmons R . Expressive motion with x, y and theta: Laban effort features for mobile robots . In: The 23rd IEEE International Symposium on Robot and Human Interactive Communication . IEEE; 2014 . p. 267 - 273 . 49. Burton SJ , Samadani AA , Gorbet R , Kuli? D. Laban movement analysis and affective movement generation for robots and other near-living creatures . In: Dance Notations and Robot Motion . Springer; 2016 . p. 25 - 48 . 50. Etemad SA , Arya A . Expert-Driven Perceptual Features for Modeling Style and Affect in Human Motion . IEEE Transactions on Human-Machine Systems . 2016 ; 46 ( 4 ): 534 - 545 . https://doi.org/10.1109/THMS. 2016 .2537760 51. Heimerdinger M , LaViers A. Modeling the Interactions of Context and Style on Affect in Motion Perception: Stylized Gaits Across Multiple Environmental Contexts . International Journal on Social Robotics . 2019 ; 1875 - 4791 : 1 - 19 . 52. Heider F , Simmel M. An experimental study of apparent behavior . The American journal of psychology . 1944 ; 57 ( 2 ): 243 - 259 . https://doi.org/10.2307/1416950 53. Cuan C , Pakrasi I , LaViers A . Time to Compile. 5th International Conference on Movement and Computing (MOCO) . 2018 ;. 54. Gouaillier D , Hugel V , Blazevic P , Kilner C , Monceaux J , Lafourcade P , et al. Mechatronic design of NAO humanoid . In: IEEE International Conference on Robotics and Automation.; 2009 . p. 769 - 774 . 55. Aldebaran . NAOqi Documentation v2 . 1: Joints . http://doc.aldebaran.com/2-1/family/robots/joints_ robot.html: SoftBank Group; 2017 . 56. Aldebaran . NAOqi Documentation v2 . 1: LEDs . http://doc.aldebaran.com/2-1/family/robots/leds_robot. html: SoftBank Group; 2017 . 57. v2 1 AND . NAO Actuator and Sensor List . http://doc.aldebaran.com/2-1/family/nao_dcm/actuator_ sensor_names.html: SoftBank Group; 2017 . 58. Echelon . Choreographed Control: Bellagio Fountains Dance With Echelon Technology . www.echelon. com; 2014 . 59. Padgett S. Fountains of Bellagio continue to amaze visitors . Las Vegas Review Journal ; 2011 . 60. Schaller RR . Moore's law: past, present and future . IEEE spectrum. 1997 ; 34 ( 6 ): 52 - 59 . 61. Ishida H , Taniguchi M. Rotary encoder apparatus . FANUC Corp ; 1985 . 62. Madni A , Vuong J , Vuong P . Pulse width modulation based digital incremental encoder . Bei Sensors and Systems Company Inc; 2004 . 63. Stephens GJ , Johnson-Kerner B , Bialek W , Ryu WS . Dimensionality and dynamics in the behavior of C. elegans . PLoS Comput Biol . 2008 ; 4 ( 4 ):e1000028. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi. 1000028 PMID: 18389066 64. Stephens GJ , Johnson-Kerner B , Bialek W , Ryu WS , Warrant E. From Modes to Movement in the Behavior of Caenorhabditis elegans . PloS one . 2010 ; 5 ( 11 ): 462 - 465 . https://doi.org/10.1371/journal. pone.0013914 65. Stephens GJ , Ryu WS , Bialek W. The emergence of stereotyped behaviors in C. elegans . Bulletin of the American Physical Society . 2010 ; 55 . 66. Gomez-Marin A , Stephens GJ , Brown AE . Hierarchical compression of Caenorhabditis elegans locomotion reveals phenotypic differences in the organization of behaviour . Journal of The Royal Society Interface . 2016 ; 13 ( 121 ): 20160466 . https://doi.org/10.1098/rsif. 2016 .0466 67. Abbott L , Kepler TB . Model neurons: from hodgkin-huxley to hopfield . In: Statistical mechanics of neural networks . Springer; 1990 . p. 5 - 18 . 68. Countryman SM , Stumpe MC , Crow SP , Adler FR , Greene MJ , Vonshak M , et al. Collective search by ants in microgravity . Frontiers in Ecology and Evolution . 2015 ; 3 : 25 . https://doi.org/10.3389/fevo. 2015 .00062 69. Azevedo FA , Carvalho LR , Grinberg LT , Farfel JM , Ferretti RE , Leite RE , et al. Equal numbers of neuronal and nonneuronal cells make the human brain an isometrically scaled-up primate brain . Journal of Comparative Neurology . 2009 ; 513 ( 5 ): 532 - 541 . https://doi.org/10.1002/cne.21974 PMID: 19226510 70. Chyb S , Gompel N. Atlas of Drosophila Morphology: Wild-type and classical mutants . Academic Press; 2013 . 71. Ting LH , Macpherson JM . A limited set of muscle synergies for force control during a postural task . Journal of neurophysiology . 2005 ; 93 ( 1 ): 609 - 613 . https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00681. 2004 PMID: 15342720 72. Range of Joint Motion Evaluation Chart . https://www.dshs.wa.gov/sites/default/files/FSA/forms/pdf/13- 585a .pdf: Washington State Department of Social and Health Services; 2014 . 73. Dankert H , Wang L , Hoopfer ED , Anderson DJ , Perona P. Automated monitoring and analysis of social behavior in Drosophila . Nature methods . 2009 ; 6 ( 4 ): 297 . https://doi.org/10.1038/nmeth.1310 PMID: 19270697 74. Sebastiani A , Fishbeck DW . Mammalian anatomy: the cat . Morton Publishing Company; 2005 . 75. Stephens GJ , Mora T , Tka?ik G , Bialek W. Statistical thermodynamics of natural images . Physical review letters . 2013 ; 110 ( 1 ): 018701 . https://doi.org/10.1103/PhysRevLett.110.018701 PMID: 23383852 76. Whitlock T. Emoji unicode tables . timwhitlock.info; 2016 . 77. Felleisen M. On the expressive power of programming languages . Science of computer programming . 1991 ; 17 ( 1-3 ): 35 - 75 . https://doi.org/10.1016/ 0167 - 6423 ( 91 ) 90036 -W 78. Veltman JA , Brunner HG . Understanding variable expressivity in microdeletion syndromes . Nature genetics . 2010 ; 42 ( 3 ): 192 . https://doi.org/10.1038/ng0310-192 PMID: 20179732 79. Restle F. Coding theory of the perception of motion configurations . Psychological Review . 1979 ; 86 ( 1 ): 1 . 80. Godfrey-Smith P . Other minds: The octopus, the sea, and the deep origins of consciousness . Farrar, Straus and Giroux; 2016 . 81. O 'Brien KW , Xu PA , Levine DJ , Aubin CA , Yang HJ , Xiao MF , et al. Elastomeric passive transmission for autonomous force-velocity adaptation applied to 3D-printed prosthetics . Science Robotics . 2018 ; 3 ( 23 ):eaau5543. https://doi.org/10.1126/scirobotics.aau5543 82. Hines L , Petersen K , Lum GZ , Sitti M. Soft Actuators for Small-Scale Robotics . Advanced Materials . 2017 ; 29 ( 13 ): 1603483 . https://doi.org/10.1002/adma.201603483 83. Mathijssen G , Schultz J , Vanderborght B , Bicchi A . A muscle-like recruitment actuator with modular redundant actuation units for soft robotics . Robotics and Autonomous Systems . 2015 ; 74 : 40 - 50 . https://doi.org/10.1016/j.robot. 2015 . 06 .010 84. Bialek W , Nemenman I , Tishby N. Predictability, complexity, and learning . Neural computation . 2001 ; 13 ( 11 ): 2409 - 2463 . https://doi.org/10.1162/089976601753195969 PMID: 11674845 85. Barry RJ , Dutton TA , Rice SA . High precision analog encoder system . Lexmark International Inc; 2000 .


This is a preview of a remote PDF: https://journals.plos.org/plosone/article/file?id=10.1371/journal.pone.0215671&type=printable

Amy LaViers. Counts of mechanical, external configurations compared to computational, internal configurations in natural and artificial systems, PLOS ONE, 2019, DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0215671